43 A.2d 145 | Pa. | 1945
This is an appeal from the entry of judgments n. o. v. in the court below. It is a railroad crossing case in which the alleged absence of due warning by the train before it reached the crossing is the basis of the complaint of negligence. The accident occurred at 6 A.M., October 27, 1942, before daylight. The minor plaintiff was a passenger in an automobile driven by John B. Kramer (the plaintiff in a companion case). Norman Kemmerer was also a passenger. He was killed in the accident. The road which these men took on the fatal morning was unfamiliar to all of them. It had but recently been reopened to traffic, after improvements had been made upon it. As the court below states in its opinion: "Neither Kramer nor Kindt knew of the existence of the railroad crossing at this point and much of the testimony was concerned with the question whether or not the rails were visible from any distance and whether the cross arm warning post was obscured by foliage and whether the words of warning were legible *421 or illegible." The parties were travelling with the windows closed. After the car was struck by a train travelling about 15 miles an hour, both Kindt and Kramer were unconscious. Before the accident both these men were looking straight ahead. Kramer testified that he heard no bell and "there was no whistle blowing." Kindt testified that "there was no train whistle sounded" and "no bell of a locomotive rung" and that they were "riding in silence."
Defendant offered the testimony of its engineer, fireman, conductor, brakeman, flagman and of another of its employes, who lived in the neighborhood, all of whom testified that the headlight was lit, with a beam showing objects 800 feet ahead, and that the whistle blew two long and two short blasts beginning at the whistle board about 1400 feet from the crossing and ending at or near the crossing. Several of the crew on the cab of the engine testified that the bell of the engine was also ringing at least up to the crossing. There were no witnesses as to the light and signals other than defendant's employees.
After trial verdicts were rendered as follows: To Harold E. Kindt as Guardian of Willard Kindt, $5,000; to Harold E. Kindt in his own right, $1172.35; to John B. Kramer, $2183.75; to Kramer's wife, Clistie M. Kramer, for damages to her automobile, $750.
The court later entered judgment in favor of the defendant n. o. v. on the ground that plaintiff's testimony in support of their allegations of lack of warning signals was "negative in quality" and since it was contradicted it "cannot sustain the verdict." The court cited the case of Knox v. P. R. Ry. Co.,
In the instant case the evidence of no warning signals was positive in form, as the excerpts earlier quoted indicate. Now the question comes to this: Was the court justified in deciding from the entire record that this evidence while positive in form was really negative in substance? We think not. The jury might have so decided under proper instructions, but we find no justification for the courts doing so.
The case of Williams v. Pittsburgh,
It is impossible to lay down any rule by which it can infallibly be determined that a witness' statement that "no whistle was blown and no bell was rung" is of so positive a character as to support a charge of negligence, or is of such negative character as to "amount only to a scintilla." The question whether such testimony is "positive" or "negative"is, except in clear cases, for the jury, for the weight to be accorded to such testimony must depend on the keenness of the witness' perceptions and the circumstances under which they functioned.
The argument is plausible that these witnesses were mistaken when they said no warnings of the approaching train were given, but the argument that a witness is mistaken can be made of almost any testimony of any witness, except as to such matters that are supported mathematically. No individual's perceptions are perfect. Cases of "mistaken identity" by honest witnesses are of common occurrence.1 *425
Two persons of equally keen hearing may be sitting in a room, and one of them will hear a noise outside while the other will not. A person habituated to intense mental concentrations is not as likely to hear noises as is one whose mind is more objective in character. It is perhaps correct to say that extroverts are more perceptive of sights and sounds than are introverts. The mind of the one is directed outward; the mind of the other is directed inward. Very frequently an introvert does not see an object he looks at. Two persons equal in vision may be sitting side by side and looking in the same direction and one of them will see objects to which the other is completely blind. The same phenomena often obtains with two persons of equal auditory powers; one will hear because his entire interest is in externals; the other will not hear because of his propensity for reflection.
The court in its charge over-simplified the issue as to whether the testimony as to the absence of warning signals and lights was positive or negative. In the charge appears this statement: "The courts have stated that testimony as to the absence of such things is called negative testimony; unless a person is on the alert, is listening for a sound or looking for a light, it is negative testimony to say that there was no sound and that there was no light, and that that amounts to saying simply that they heard no sound or that they saw no light." To say that unless a person "is listening for a sound or looking *426 for a light" his testimony as to the absence of a sound or a light "is negative testimony" and so entitled to but little weight is to ignore reality. It is reasonable to believe that if a person is of normal alertness he will hear the whistle of a nearby locomotive unless he is absorbed in thought or conversation. When a witness testifies, as did Kindt and Kramer, that there was "no whistle blown" and "no train whistle sounded," they should be examined and cross-examined as to their alertness and attentiveness at the time. When Kramer's counsel asked him after he had testified "there was no whistle blown", "Would you have heard one if there would have been one blown?" the court sustained an objection to the question, saying: "He has already said there was no whistle blown." This statement naturally halted the attorney in his development of facts as to the witness' alertness or attentiveness, which facts might have added to the probative value of the statement.
The fact that a witness of keen hearing, under circumstances favorable to hearing a locomotive whistle, "heard no whistle"may have more probative value than the statement of a witness that "no whistle was blown" if it develops that the latter's hearing was impaired or that he was mentally preoccupied at the time or that the circumstances in which he was placed were unfavorable to his hearing a whistle. The probative value of a truthful witness' statement as to what he saw or heard depends on its factual background and on his sensory powers and the accuracy of his memory. Judging the credibility of a witness and the weight of his testimony is preeminently a jury's function. Of course, when the testimony offered does not evenif credited meet the burden resting upon a plaintiff, it is the trial judge's duty so to declare. If in the instant case the jury accepted at its face value the testimony that no whistle was blown or bell rung, plaintiffs' burden on that phase of the case was met.
Appellee contended that the testimony in question meant only that the witnesses did not hear the signals; *427
and this contention the court accepted as an inference from the fact that the witnesses did not say they were consciouslylistening for signals at the time. Drawing inference from facts is a jury function. We said in MacDonald v. P. R. R. Co.,
In the instant case, the court in declaring the testimony referred to as "negative" went further than our cases warrant. In Rottmund v. Penna. R. R. Co.,
The foregoing cases indicate that a trial judge is not warranted in characterizing as "negative" the testimony of witnesses who in a grade crossing case stated unequivocally that no whistle was blown or bell rung unless there are attendant circumstances which make it reasonable to believe that their testimony, though positive in form, is only negative in substance, as e. g., when it is reasonable to believe that a witness was so situated or so self-absorbed that he would not have heard the bell or the whistle though for a certainty it was blown. For example, a statement by a very deaf man or by a man completely absorbed in thought at the time, that no whistle was blown would simply mean that he did not hear a whistle blown. When witnesses testify that no whistle was blown or bell rung, all the circumstances which surrounded the witnesses at the time and something as to the perceptive faculties of the witnesses, should be brought out either on direct or on cross-examination, and the jury should decide whether or not their testimony is of a positive and convincing character. "The tone and manner of a witness not infrequently indicate whether or not he is telling the truth": Belmont Laboratories, Inc. v.Heist et al.,
If the testimony of those who declared that no whistle was blown or bell rung is discredited by the circumstances under which the witnesses' perceptive faculties functioned or by the dullness of those faculties and is totally at variance with the testimony of those who gave the warning signals and of those who were in a position to hear such signals and who said theydid hear them, and if the jury's verdict is manifestly in capricious disregard of the overwhelming weight of the evidence, such a verdict may be set aside, as we pointed out in the MacDonald case, supra.
The judgments for the defendant n. o. v. are reversed. The rules for new trials in each case are reinstated and made absolute.
The fallability of all human testimony is further illustrated by the divergent reports of the reception accorded Lincoln's Gettysburg Address. Professor John L. Haney in his monograph on that address said: "The testimony of those who were present is to the effect that it was received: (1) with indifference on the part of the tired and inattentive spectators; (2) in silence, with no applause during or after the delivery; (3) with applause in the middle and at the end; (4) with tears, sobs, and cheers; (5) with wild and lengthened excitement; (6) with remarkable enthusiasm; (7) with loud outbursts of applause; (8) with roars of applause; (9) with hurricanes of applause." There was a similar divergence in the reports as to Lincoln's use or non-use of a manuscript on that occasion, and as to the quality of his voice. Some reported his voice as being "thin and high"; others, as "deep and powerful." Some said he "could be heard by all"; others that "he could not be heard, he spoke so low."
Professor Hugo Munsterberg in his book "On the Witness Stand," discusses, inter alia, the "illusions" of witnesses and after describing certain experiments he made testing the perceptions of his students in psychology at Harvard University, says: "Since I saw that my own students do not know whether . . . a [certain] time interval is half a second or a whole minute; whether there are twenty-five points or two hundred [on a large sheet of white cardboard exposed for five seconds and on which fifty small black squares were pasted]; whether a tone comes from a whistle, a gong, or a violin; . . . I am not surprised any more when I read the reports of the papers [on conflicting evidence offered]."